Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Relationships and Sexual Health Attractiveness

Unattractive faces may get a break: Study reveals “ugly leniency effect” in guilt judgments

by Eric W. Dolan
February 23, 2025
in Attractiveness, Social Psychology
[Imagen 3]

[Imagen 3]

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A surprising study published in Psychiatry, Psychology and Law suggests that when people make judgments about a defendant’s guilt in a case where physical appearance could plausibly play a role—such as a blind date swindle—they tend to be more lenient toward unattractive faces. This finding, which the researchers describe as an “ugly leniency effect,” challenges the assumption that attractive individuals always receive more favorable treatment.

The researchers set out to understand how first impressions based on facial appearance might affect decisions of guilt or innocence in legal cases. They were particularly interested in the roles of facial attractiveness and trustworthiness when a defendant’s appearance could be used as an advantage in committing a crime.

Previous work had shown that attractive people are often assumed to possess positive traits, while unattractive individuals may be unfairly judged harshly. However, the connection between attractiveness and guilt had not been fully explored, especially in cases where the appearance itself might help the crime succeed.

“I was really interested in exploring the consequences of facial biases in the legal field,” said study author Antonio Olivera-La Rosa, a full professor at Luis Amigó Catholic University in Medellín. “I feel that, among all the areas susceptible to experiencing the consequences of facial biases, the legal field is one that requires the most attention. It is not difficult to understand why. Personally, I think it’s important to integrate our knowledge of human cognition to human practices and institutions.”

For their study, the researchers recruited 128 participants through email and social media. The participants were primarily from Colombia, with a smaller number from Spain and Peru. The study was conducted online, and all participants provided their consent before starting.

First, participants completed a questionnaire to collect demographic information, such as age, gender, and education level. They also completed the Rational Experiential Inventory, a tool used to measure individual differences in thinking styles. This inventory assesses the degree to which individuals rely on rational thinking (analytical, intentional) versus experiential thinking (intuitive, automatic).

Next, participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups: a high time pressure condition or a low time pressure condition. In the high time pressure condition, participants were instructed to make their judgments as quickly as possible. In the low time pressure condition, they were given as much time as they needed.

All participants were then presented with a hypothetical scenario: a blind date swindle case. They were asked to imagine themselves as judges and to assess the guilt or innocence of nine male faces. The faces were selected from the Chicago Face Database and categorized into three levels of attractiveness: unattractive, neutral, and attractive. Importantly, all faces had consistent and average levels of perceived trustworthiness. Participants were asked to give their first impression of whether or not each face looked guilty. After each judgment, they rated how certain they were about their decision on a seven-point scale.

The results of the study revealed a significant effect of facial attractiveness on guilt judgments. Specifically, participants were more likely to judge unattractive faces as innocent compared to both neutral and attractive faces. This finding suggests that an “ugly leniency effect” was at play, where less attractive individuals were given the benefit of the doubt.

The researchers found that time pressure did not significantly influence the relationship between facial attractiveness and guilt judgments. Whether participants were under time constraints or not, the ugly leniency effect persisted.

They also found that individual differences in thinking styles, as measured by the Rational Experiential Inventory, did not significantly impact guilt judgments. Participants who were more inclined toward rational thinking were just as likely to exhibit the ugly leniency effect as those who were more inclined toward experiential thinking.

The study further explored participants’ beliefs about the role of physical appearance in the crime scenario. Researchers found that participants generally believed that the defendant’s appearance could influence the success or failure of the swindle. Additionally, a majority of participants acknowledged that the attractiveness or unattractiveness of the faces may have influenced their responses.

“Our results provide valuable insights by demonstrating that, under specific conditions, unattractive faces can reduce guilt perception,” Olivera-La Rosa told PsyPost. “This evidence could enhance awareness of how implicit bias influences legal decisions, potentially reducing the skepticism many judges express towards such extralegal factors. While the legal system is traditionally grounded in rationality, it is not immune to the influence of facial biases.”

But there is an important caveat to consider: the study focused on a specific type of crime—a blind date swindle. This focus limits the generalizability of the findings to other types of offenses.

“The purpose of our study was to gain a deeper insight into the intuitive nature of initial judgments about guilt or innocence,” Olivera-La Rosa explained. “As such, our findings should not be seen as proof that individuals with unattractive (yet moderately trustworthy) faces are necessarily more likely to receive lenient judicial sentences. Indeed, facial bias may be reduced or even diminished during the examination of evidence.”

Future research could examine the effects of facial attractiveness on guilt judgments in a wider range of criminal scenarios, using both male and female faces. The researchers aim to further explore how factors such as facial appearance can unconsciously influence jurors’ decisions and to develop methods for reducing these biases within the legal system.

“The progress made within the interdisciplinary field known as neurolaw has led to greater interest in studying these issues from perspectives that incorporate cognitive sciences,” Olivera-La Rosa said. “Although it may sound obvious, jurors are still people, which means that they can be susceptible to facial bias. More research is definitely needed, along with identifying the best ways to incorporate these findings into traditional legal systems.”

The study, “When being unattractive is an advantage: effects of face perception on intuitive culpability judgments,” was authored by Antonio Olivera-La Rosa, Luis D. Ayala, and Ricardo M. Tamayo.

RELATED

Exposure to smartphone light suppresses melatonin levels at night
Anxiety

Night owls more prone to problematic smartphone use, with loneliness and anxiety as key factors

September 22, 2025
Study identifies predictors of women’s psychological well-being in romantic relationships
Relationships and Sexual Health

Marriage provides little additional life satisfaction boost beyond that of partnership and cohabitation

September 22, 2025
Lower creatine intake associated with greater depression and anxiety
Social Psychology

Individuals perceiving their social status as higher tend to be worse at perceiving emotions of others

September 22, 2025
Psychology researchers uncover how personality relates to rejection of negative feedback
Political Psychology

A 40-year study finds higher science funding under Republicans

September 21, 2025
Psychology researchers uncover how personality relates to rejection of negative feedback
Mental Health

Genetic analysis upends narrative linking social media use to lower wellbeing

September 21, 2025
Psychology researchers uncover how personality relates to rejection of negative feedback
Social Psychology

Psychology researchers uncover how personality relates to rejection of negative feedback

September 21, 2025
Social contagion is powerful — here’s how researchers suggest we can build resistance
Social Psychology

Social contagion is powerful — here’s how researchers suggest we can build resistance

September 20, 2025
Verbal IQ predicts political participation and liberal attitudes twice as strongly as performance IQ
Cognitive Science

Politics and IQ: Are liberals smarter than conservatives?

September 20, 2025

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Night owls more prone to problematic smartphone use, with loneliness and anxiety as key factors

Personality traits predict students’ use of generative AI in higher education, study finds

Marriage provides little additional life satisfaction boost beyond that of partnership and cohabitation

Study finds caffeine increases task persistence under pressure

Individuals perceiving their social status as higher tend to be worse at perceiving emotions of others

PTSD symptoms linked to the tendency to absorb others’ stress reactions

Common cinnamon metabolite shows potential for Alzheimer’s treatment by reducing amyloid in blood

Lower creatine intake associated with greater depression and anxiety

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy